تأثیر تمرینات حیاط پویا بر تبحرحرکتی و کارکردهای اجرایی دختران دارای اضافه وزن

نوع مقاله : مقاله پژوهشی

نویسنده

گروه تربیت بدنی و علوم ورزشی، واحد همدان، دانشگاه آزاد اسلامی، همدان، ایران.

چکیده

هدف
تاثیر زندگی مدرن موجب افزایش مدت زمان نشستن در کودکان و کاهش فعالیت بدنی گردیده است. یکی از در دسترس­ترین امکانات در هر مدرسه یا فضای عمومی شهری ایجاد حیاط پویا است. هدف این مطالعه بررسی تاثیر 32 جلسه تمرینات حیاط پویا بر عملکرد حرکتی و شناختی دختران ده ساله دارای اضافه وزن بود.
روش پژوهش
شرکت­کنندگان این مطالعه شامل 30 دختر ده ساله دارای اضافه وزن بود که بر اساس شاخص توده بدنی (BMI) و به صورت تصادفی از مدارس شهر همدان انتخاب شدند. عملکرد جسمانی این افراد با استفاده از آزمون برونینکس-ازوروتسکی (BOT-2) ارزیابی شد. کارکردهای اجرایی نیز با استفاده از آزمون استروپ و زمان واکنش ساده مورد سنجش واقع گردید. برای مقایسه نتایج از آزمون تحلیل کوواریانس استفاده شد.
یافته‌ها
طبق نتایج، تمرینات حیاط پویا توانست موجب افزایش معنی­دار در خرده مقیاس­های سرعت دویدن و تعادل گردید (05/0>p). همچنین بعد از تمرینات، افزایش تعداد پاسخ­های صحیح و کاهش تعداد محرک­های بی­پاسخ در هر دو آزمون زمان وااکنش و آزمون استروپ، مشاهده گردید (05/0>p).
نتیجه گیری
نتایج این مطالعه نشان داد، تمرینات حیاط پویا می‌تواند به‌طور قابل توجهی اجزای کارکردهای اجرایی و مهارت‌های حرکتی را در دختران ده‌ساله دارای اضافه وزن بهبود بخشد. این تمرینات با ادغام چالش‌های شناختی و حرکتی در یک محیط جذاب و تعاملی موجب افزایش فعالیت­های جسمانی و در نتیجه تاثیر مثبت بر عملکرد شناختی و حرکتی این کودکان داشته است.

کلیدواژه‌ها

موضوعات


عنوان مقاله [English]

The Effect of Dynamic Yard training on Motor Skills and Executive Functions of Overweight Girls

نویسنده [English]

  • Elaheh Azadian
Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Hamedan Branch, Islamic Azad University, Hamedan, Iran.
چکیده [English]

Objective: The impact of modern life has led to increased sedentary behavior and reduced physical activity among children. One of the most accessible solutions in schools or urban public spaces is the creation of dynamic yard. This study aimed to investigate the effects of 32 sessions of dynamic yard training on the motor and cognitive performance of overweight 10-year-old girls.
Method: Participants included 30 overweight 10-year-old girls selected randomly from schools in Hamedan based on their Body Mass Index (BMI). Physical performance was assessed using the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency (BOT-2), while executive functions were evaluated using the Stroop Test and Simple Reaction Time Test. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to compare the results.
Results: The findings indicated that dynamic playground exercises significantly improved subscales of running speed and balance (p < 0.05). Additionally, after the intervention, there was an increase in the number of correct responses and a decrease in the number of unanswered stimuli in both the reaction time and Stroop tests (p < 0.05).
Conclusions: The results of this study suggest that dynamic playground exercises can significantly enhance components of executive functions and motor skills in overweight 10-year-old girls. By integrating cognitive and motor challenges in an engaging and interactive environment, these exercises promote physical activity and positively impact cognitive and motor performance in children.

کلیدواژه‌ها [English]

  • Overweight
  • Motor performance
  • Executive function
  • Dynamic yard

Introduction

The increasing prevalence of childhood obesity has emerged as a serious public health concern, with profound implications for children’s overall health and development (1). Recent research suggests that the increasing rates of childhood obesity are linked to a variety of lifestyle factors, including decreased physical activity and increased sedentary behaviors, that have become more prevalent in modern society (2). According to the World Health Organization (WHO), the global prevalence of childhood obesity has increased from 4% in 1975 to approximately 18% in recent years, affecting more than 340 million children and adolescents aged 5–19 years worldwide (3, 4).

Research suggests that obese children are more likely to have deficits in both gross and fine motor skills compared to their normal-weight peers (5). This decline in motor skill performance can limit their ability to participate in physical activities, creating a cycle of inactivity and weight gain (6). Studies have also shown that mechanical limitations caused by excess weight can have a negative impact on motor control and coordination (7-10), balance and agility (11), and in addition, reduce self-esteem and increase social isolation (12).

In addition, some studies have provided evidence that indicates a negative association between excess weight and cognitive function (13). These associations have also been observed with different cognitive domains such as learning and memory abilities (14). Cognitive functions, especially executive functions, are closely related to motor performance. Individuals with severe obesity have shown poorer performance in tasks that require motor planning, processing speed, attention and inhibition (15). The integration of cognitive and motor functions depends on an extensive network in the frontal region of the brain that is essential for performing various executive functions. Disturbances in this network can lead to problems in cognitive and motor functions (16). However, some studies have reported conflicting evidence that, according to the “obesity paradigm,” obese individuals may perform similarly or better than normal-weight individuals in executive functions (17).

Perceptual-motor skills are a complex and multidimensional set of developmental abilities in children that have two main aspects: perception and movement. The acquisition of these abilities is an important element in successful athletic performance (18). The quality of motor performance depends on sensory integration, which is influenced by the correct perception of information from the environment and the ability of the individual to interpret and integrate this information into the nervous system. Sensory integration is a complex neural process whose final product is known as the basis for motor and perceptual development in children (19). Perceptual-motor skills are influenced by many factors such as age, gender, maturity, family socioeconomic status, etc. Some studies have shown that opportunities for children to practice and exercise have an impact on the development of perceptual-motor skills, and therefore these skills can be taught and improved (18).

Some studies on the effects of motor training have shown that interventions that focus on increasing physical activity levels and developing motor skills can improve motor skills in overweight children by creating a supportive environment. Some studies have shown that structured exercise interventions that focus on fundamental motor skills, such as moving and controlling objects, have produced significant improvements in motor abilities in overweight children (20, 21). Other studies include dynamic and static balance exercises, which have shown a significant positive effect on balance skills in these individuals (22). The use of active video games has also been effective in improving specific motor skills, such as throwing and catching, in overweight children (23).

Despite these findings, there is still little research on the effects of exercise programs on the motor and cognitive development of overweight children, especially during childhood. This study intends to use environmental interventions with an emphasis on games available in a dynamic yard. In a dynamic yard, purposeful shapes are designed on the ground to practice fundamental motor skills and also develop physical fitness of students (24, 25). Therefore, this study was conducted to investigate the effects of a dynamic yard on perceptual-motor skills and cognitive performance of 10-year-old overweight children.

 

Method: The present study was a semi-experimental study conducted in the field. The research design included a pre-test and post-test using two experimental and control groups. Participants: The statistical population included all overweight female students in the fourth grade of elementary school studying in schools in Hamedan. Using G-Power software with a power of 80% and an alpha of 0.05, the minimum number of students in each group was estimated to be 30 (26). Among all elementary schools, two schools were selected in a multi-stage cluster. Then, 15 students from one school were selected as the experimental group and 15 students from the other school were selected as the control group. The inclusion criteria for both groups were age of about 10 years and being overweight, and the exclusion criteria were inability to perform the pre-post-test, the presence of diseases and neuromuscular and orthopedic problems that affect the execution of movements. The consent form was signed by the parents of the students.

 

Results: The results of the BOT-2 test are shown in Table 3. According to the results of the covariance test, the implementation of dynamic yard exercises led to improvements in some perceptual-motor skills. According to the results, the experimental group showed significant improvements in the subscales of running speed and agility (p=0.035), balance (p=0.046), and total score (p=0.042) after the exercises. The results of the covariance test after adjusting the means showed that the dynamic yard exercises caused a decrease in response time (p=0.018) and a decrease of more than 100 percent in the number of no-response stimuli (p<0.001) in the experimental group. Also, in the subscale of compatible stimuli of the Stroop test, after the exercises, the number of correct responses increased by about 34 percent (p=0.015) and the number of no-response stimuli decreased by 38 percent (p=0.009). In the incongruent stimuli subscale, the number of correct responses increased by about 25% (p=0.034) and the number of unresponsive stimuli decreased by about 37% (p=0.012). The interference score also decreased significantly by about 60% (p=0.008).

 

Conclusion: The findings of this study indicate that dynamic training in the school yard significantly improved motor skills in overweight 10-year-old girls. These improvements were observed in running speed and balance, as well as in the overall score of the BOT-2 test. The improvement in balance, in particular, indicates the potential of dynamic training in the yard to enhance postural control and stability, which are critical for general motor skills and injury prevention.

The results of this study are consistent with previous research that has shown the benefits of physical activity for the development of motor skills in children. Some of these studies have shown that structured physical activity programs can improve balance, coordination, and agility in overweight and obese children (20, 21). The findings of this study on the effect of dynamic training were also consistent with the results of Babakhani et al. (2022) (25). The significant improvements in running speed and balance after dynamic yard exercises suggest that these activities may promote the development of motor skills by providing a sensorimotor-rich environment. These activities typically involve varied and unpredictable movements that may help develop motor planning, coordination, and adaptability. These findings are consistent with the principles of ecological dynamics, which emphasize the importance of task variety and interaction with the environment in learning skills. From an ecological perspective, all experiences that children encounter from birth contribute to strengthening the person-environment relationship and are considered learning experiences (28). The school environment plays a significant role in providing constructive learning opportunities that increase cognitive flexibility and prepare students to effectively deal with the most complex tasks (29).

The findings of this study on the effect of dynamic yard exercises indicated a significant effect of these exercises on executive functions. The increase in the number of correct responses indicates an improvement in decision-making and planning efficiency, and the significant reduction in unresponsive stimuli may indicate the effect of the exercises on increasing the concentration of these individuals. These factors are especially important for overweight children, because according to previous studies, this group often faces deficits in executive function, including reduced information processing speed and concentration and attention (30, 31). These results were in line with studies that emphasize the role of physical activity on cognitive and motor performance in overweight children (32). The results of the Stroop test also confirm the cognitive benefits of dynamic yard exercises. The increase in correct responses and the reduction in unresponsive stimuli in both the congruent and incongruent stimuli subscales indicate improvements in selective attention, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control. The significant reduction in the interference score may also indicate a reduction in the impact of contradictory information on movement execution and indicates an improvement in the ability to manage cognitive interference. These findings are consistent with the principles of cognitive-motor integration, which states that physical activities that require complex movements and decision-making can stimulate neural pathways associated with executive functions (33, 34). A dynamic playground is an environment that is constantly available to children without the need for sports equipment. In general, enriching children's environments in terms of sensory, social, and physical stimuli provides new experiences, mental and motor challenges, and opportunities for interaction and learning. Research has shown that exposure to such environments can have a positive effect on brain development and improve cognitive and motor functions (35). One mechanism that can explain these changes in the nervous system is increased neuroplasticity, which means the creation of new neural connections and strengthening existing networks in the brain. This process helps the brain learn better and adapt to new conditions (36).

 

Keywords: Dynamic yard, Executive function, Motor performance, Overweight,

 

 

Ethical Considerations

 

Compliance with ethical guidelines

This study was conducted with applied goals and in compliance with all research guidelines and ethical principles regarding participants, including informed voluntary consent, the right to withdraw from the study if desired, and the protection of confidential information of the subjects.

 

Funding

No financial support was received from public, commercial, or non-governmental funding sources during this study.

 

Authors' contribution

All authors contributed equally to the conceptualization of the article and writing of the original and subsequent drafts.

 

Conflict of interest

The authors declared no conflict of interest.

 

 

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all participants of the present study.

 

  1. Sheikholeslami-Vatani D, Jahani N. The effect of rope training on physical fitness parameters in 9-12 years old overweight/obese boys. Journal of Practical Studies of Biosciences in Sport. 2014;2(3):60-71.
  2. Colley RC, Garriguet D, Adamo KB, Carson V, Janssen I, Timmons BW, et al. Physical activity and sedentary behavior during the early years in Canada: a cross-sectional study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2013;10:1-9.
  3. Hasan HA. Child and adolescent obesity prevalence in the Gulf Cooperation Council countries and its impact on adolescent females in Kuwait. 2021.
  4. Organization WH. The double burden of malnutrition: priority actions on ending childhood obesity: World Health Organization. Regional Office for South-East Asia; 2020.
  5. Zacks B, Confroy K, Frino S, Skelton JA. Delayed motor skills associated with pediatric obesity. Obesity Research & Clinical Practice. 2021;15(1):1-9.
  6. Gil Madrona P, Romero Martinez SJ, Sáez-Gallego NM, Ordóñez Camacho XG. Psychomotor limitations of overweight and obese five-year-old children: Influence of body mass indices on motor, perceptual, and social-emotional skills. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019;16(3):427.
  7. Liang J, Matheson B, Kaye W, Boutelle K. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. International journal of obesity. 2014;38(4):494-506.
  8. Jiménez‐Pavón D, Kelly J, Reilly JJ. Associations between objectively measured habitual physical activity and adiposity in children and adolescents: Systematic review. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity. 2010;5(1):3-18.
  9. Roshandel Hesari A, Roshandel Hesari A, Mohagheghi Pirshahid A. Comparison of Musculoskeletal Disorders in the Upper Limbs of Male Athletes with Obese and Thin Non-Athlete Males. Journal of Sport Biomechanics. 2023;9(1):32-46.
  10. Najafian Razavi M. The Comparison of Gait Kinematics in Over-Weight and Normal-Weight People across Age Groups. Journal of Sport Biomechanics. 2022;8(3):214-30.
  11. Barros WMA, Silva KGd, Silva RKP, Souza APdS, Silva ABJd, Silva MRM, et al. Effects of overweight/obesity on motor performance in children: a systematic review. Frontiers in endocrinology. 2022;12:759165.
  12. Sagar R, Gupta T. Psychological aspects of obesity in children and adolescents. The Indian Journal of Pediatrics. 2018;85:554-9.
  13. Balasubramanian P, Kiss T, Tarantini S, Nyúl-Tóth Á, Ahire C, Yabluchanskiy A, et al. Obesity-induced cognitive impairment in older adults: a microvascular perspective. American Journal of Physiology-Heart and Circulatory Physiology. 2021;320(2):H740-H61.
  14. Klupp S, Möhring W, Lemola S, Grob A. Relations between fine motor skills and intelligence in typically developing children and children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research in developmental disabilities. 2021;110:103855.
  15. Appelhans BM, French SA, Pagoto SL, Sherwood NE. Managing temptation in obesity treatment: A neurobehavioral model of intervention strategies. Appetite. 2016;96:268-79.
  16. Du Z, Li J, Huang J, Ma J, Xu X, Zou R, et al. Executive functions in predicting weight loss and obesity indicators: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;11:604113.
  17. La Marra M, Villano I, Ilardi CR, Carosella M, Staiano M, Iavarone A, et al. Executive functions in overweight and obese treatment-seeking patients: Cross-sectional data and longitudinal perspectives. Brain Sciences. 2022;12(6):777.
  18. Rice MS, Rizzardo B, Warburton DE, Bredin SS. Developing skilled motor performance: A systematic review of perceptual skill training in children and youth. Journal of Exercise, Movement, and Sport (SCAPPS refereed abstracts repository). 2019;51(1):50-.
  19. Wuang Y-P, Huang C-L, Tsai H-Y. Sensory integration and perceptual-motor profiles in school-aged children with autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. 2020;16:1661.
  20. Cliff DP, Wilson A, Okely AD, Mickle KJ, Steele JR. Feasibility of SHARK: a physical activity skill-development program for overweight and obese children. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2007;10(4):263-7.
  21. Korsten-Reck U, Kaspar T, Korsten K, Kromeyer-Hauschild K, Bös K, Berg A, et al. Motor abilities and aerobic fitness of obese children. International journal of sports medicine. 2007;28(09):762-7.
  22. Sola K, Brekke N, Brekke M. An activity-based intervention for obese and physically inactive children organized in primary care: feasibility and impact on fitness and BMI: A one-year follow-up study. Scandinavian journal of primary health care. 2010;28(4):199-204.
  23. Van Biljon A, Longhurst G. The influence of exergaming on the functional fitness in overweight and obese children: Physical activity, health and wellness. African Journal for Physical Health Education, Recreation and Dance. 2012;18(42):984-91.
  24. Ridgers ND, Stratton G, McKenzie TL. Reliability and validity of the System for Observing Children’s Activity and Relationships during Play (SOCARP). Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2010;7(1):17-25.
  25. Babakhani M, Namazizadeh M, Mousavi Sadati SK, Daneshfar A. The effect of dynamic yard on motor development of 8 to 12 year old students. medical journal of mashhad university of medical sciences. 2022;64(6):4214-23.
  26. Dehghan Nasab A, Azadian E. Relationship between Fundamental Movement Skills and Variability in Postural Control: Comparison of Children with and without Intellectual Disability. Pajouhan Scientific Journal. 2024;22(1):31-41.
  27. De Waal E, Pienaar AE. Influences of persistent overweight on perceptual-motor proficiency of primary school children: The North-West CHILD longitudinal study: Persistent overweight and perceptual-motor proficiency in children. BMC pediatrics. 2021;21(1):245.
  28. Davids K, Rothwell M, Rudd J. Enriching lives across the lifescourse: adopting an ecological perspective to foster the person-environment relationship throughout development. Universidade do Algarve-Escola Superior de Educação e Comunicação. 2023:21-8.
  29. D'Anna C, Coppola S, Vastola R. Transfer goal: real task in physical education as representative learning environment. GIORNALE ITALIANO DI EDUCAZIONE ALLA SALUTE, SPORT E DIDATTICA INCLUSIVA. 2022;6.(1).
  30. Mazzoccante R, de Luca Corrêa H, de Santana FS, Câmara MA, de Sousa BC, de Sousa IC, et al. Attention and executive function are predicted by anthropometric indicators, strength, motor performance and aerobic fitness in children aged 6 to 10 years. Human Movement. 2020;21(1):40-8.
  31. Emery RL, Levine MD. Questionnaire and behavioral task measures of impulsivity are differentially associated with body mass index: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin. 2017;143(8):868.
  32. Wang Y, Wang H, Zhao H. Effects of aerobic exercise on executive function among overweight and obese children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology. 2024;15:1485610.
  33. Tomporowski PD, Lambourne K, Okumura MS. Physical activity interventions and children's mental function: an introduction and overview. Preventive medicine. 2011;52:S3-S9.
  34. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature reviews neuroscience. 2008;9(1):58-65.
  35. Sale A, Berardi N, Maffei L. Environment and brain plasticity: towards an endogenous pharmacotherapy. Physiological reviews. 2014;94(1):189-234.
  36. Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ. Enriched environments, experience-dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7(9):697-709.